L2 Study Strategies - Institutes

Study 2 we're working on now...

Home
Study Strategies and Institutes
Contact Us
Future Research
Under Construction!

Running Head: Foreign Language Study Strategy

 

 

 

Foreign Language Study Strategy Use and Language Institute Attendance

by Korean EFL and CFL University Students

 

 

Gregory C. Brundage B.A.

Konyang University, S. Korea

 

Deng Kui, M.S.

Nankai University, China

 

 

 

Contact information:

Gregory C. Brundage    Konyang University, Department of Foreign Language Education

Home address:                101-1003 Buyong Apt.

Nonsan City, Chungchongnam Province

South Korea 320-751

e-mail:               greg_brundage@yahoo.com

Home telephone:            (82) 041-735-1926

Mobile phone:   (82) 019-602-5099

 

 

 

 

Key words: Study strategies, Korean, EFL, CFL, Language Institutes

 

 

Abstract

This paper compares foreign language study strategies as measured by the Strategic Inventory for Language Learning (SILL) used by Korean university students (N = 276) who study either English as a Foreign Language (EFL) or Chinese as a Foreign Language (CFL), and also have attended private foreign language institutes, to study strategies used by university students that haven’t attended private foreign language institutes. Relationships between foreign language study strategy use, institute attendance and gender, educational levels of parents, self-reported self confidence, and foreign language class grades earned in the university are also examined. Results showed students that have attended private language institutes use significantly more study strategies than students that haven’t attended institutes. Results are discussed in light of past and current research.

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Ti: Foreign Language Study Strategy Use and Language Institute Attendance by Korean EFL and CFL University Students

 

 

I. INTRODUCTION

Though relationships between foreign language study strategies and many factors have been examined in previous research, the relationship between study strategies and private language institute attendance has not been considered. This paper examines that relationship in three university samples in South Korea, as well as several other factors influencing foreign language study strategy use.

 

 

1. Korea and private language institutes

Koreans have an unbridled enthusiasm for education with roots wide and deep in its history and culture. Pertinent Confucian aphorisms abound: “Ignorance is the night of the mind; a night without moon or stars,” “The scholar who cherishes the love of comfort cannot be deemed a scholar,” and “Learning without thought is labor lost; thought without learning is perilous.” These beliefs lie at the foundation of Korean mentality on this subject. Confucian philosophy reinforces the concepts that “man is perfectible thorough education,” “only the educated should rule,” and “knowledge enhances moral governance.”

 

 

   During the past 20 years South Korea has catapulted to world leadership in many domains. The Secretary General of the United Nations is Korean. Korean students rank first in the world in problem solving skills, and year after year score in the top three on math and science aptitude tests (U.S. Department of Education, 2007). In 2006 South Korea even usurped perennial first place winner Finland in reading (OECD Executive Summary 2007). The South Korean economy is ranked 13th in the world according to GDP (World Bank, 2008). In 1945, 75% of Koreans were illiterate.  In the year 2000, South Korea had a near 100% literacy rate.

 

   In 2000, 58% of Korean students attended private after-school institutes. By 2003 that percentage had jumped to 72%. It is estimated that Koreans spend 15 trillion Won ($14.4 billion dollars) each year on private English language education (Song, 2008).

 

 

   Though South Koreans have promoted equal educational opportunity since independence in 1948, this is hard to maintain, given that more affluent parents can afford highly skilled tutors and private institutes for their children while less affluent parents can not. This trend has undermined the common people’s confidence in the educational system, based on the assumption that standard public and private school education isn’t adequate to sufficiently prepare students for Korea’s hyper-competitive society. Students compete to get into elite middle schools, repeat the process to get into elite high schools, and repeat the process again to get into top universities.

 

 

   However, according to a 2003 report by the Hong Kong based Political and Economic Risk Consultancy, South Korea ranked lowest among 12 countries in East Asia for communication in English (cited in “Does Korea,” 2007). 

 

 

   In spite of the enormity of this ever-growing industry in South Korea, very little research has been done on the educational, social or psychological effects on students associated with private language academy attendance. 

 

 

   This study looks at the relationship between English as a Foreign Language (EFL), and Chinese as a Foreign Language (CFL) university students’ attendance at private language institutes and their use of study strategies as measured by the Strategic Inventory of Language Learning (SILL, Oxford, 1990), as well as the relationships between these factors and gender, parents’ education levels, self-reported self-confidence, and average grades earned in university foreign language classes.

 

2. The Strategic Inventory for Language Learning (SILL)

Numerous studies have shown that proficient language learners use a wider range of learning strategies than those who are less proficient in language learning (Ruben, 1975; Oxford, 1985, 1989, 1990; Green & Oxford, 1995, cited in Oxford, 1996; Wharton, 2000; Altan, 2004.

 

 

   The most commonly used study strategy inventory at this time is the SILL, Version 7.0 (Oxford, 1990). Numerous studies have found its reliability to be between .91 and .95 (Oxford 1996). The SILL, originally organized by a factor analytic system, divides L2 study strategies into six broad categories: memory, cognitive (using all mental processes), compensation (filling in missing knowledge), metacognitive (organizing and evaluating learning), affective (managing emotions), and social learning with others.

 

 

   Extensive research supports the proposition that numerous variables influence a learner’s choice of strategy, including motivation, gender, type of task, age, subject matter, level of L2, learning style and cultural background (Oxford, 1989). Because there are sometimes dramatic inconsistencies in reported results on study strategy use and the aforementioned factors, other variables need to be considered. Two such potential factors are parental education level and learning experiences outside of school. Preliminary research by Hsu (2003, 2008) found there are significant differences in study strategy use by students with more highly educated parents, and that students with parents who have more education tend to adopt cognitive and social strategies more often. Also, learning experience outside of school fosters the use of more cognitive and compensation strategies.

 

 

a. Research on Memory and L2 study

There are nine questions on the SILL relating to memory strategies. Phillips (1990, 1991, cited in Oxford, 2006) found a strong relationship between English language proficiency and TOEFL scores, and that low scorers (generally beginning students) relied heavily on memory (e.g. flash cards), affective, and social strategies. However, Green (1992, cited in Oxford, 1996) found no significant differences in the use of memory and affective strategies at different proficiency levels as measured by ESLAT scores.

 

 

   In a study by Chang, (cited in Yang, 2007), memory strategies were found to be least used by Taiwanese students studying in the U.S.  Similar results were found among Chinese students in Taiwan (Yang, 1993a, 1993b, cited in Yang, 2007) and Korean students (Oh, 1992).

 

 

b. Cognitive approaches to L2 study

Cognitive strategies are broadly defined as those “Using all mental processes” and include such strategies as: using English words in many different ways, watching TV and movies in English, writing, finding patterns, interpreting rather than just translating English, and making summaries.

 

 

   Studying a group of 325 Korean secondary students, Lee (2003) found that cognitive strategies correlated strongly with meta-cognitive and memory strategies. First-year secondary school students were found to use more metacognitive, cognitive and memory strategies, whereas third-year students were found to use more compensation and memory strategies.

 

 

c. Compensation strategies and L2 study

Compensation strategies are used for making up for missing knowledge and include making guesses about unknown words, using gestures and substituting words that are known for words that a student may not know.

 

 

   Hsu (2003) found compensatory strategies to be most used by junior-college students, and least used by elementary students in Taiwan. Likewise, Yang (2007) found compensation strategies were most used by junior college students in Taiwan, followed by social, cognitive, metacognitive, affective and memory strategies.

 

 

d.  Metacognitive strategies and L2 study

Metacognitive strategies include noticing speaking mistakes, paying attention when someone is speaking English, looking for people with whom to speak English, setting goals and thinking about progress.

 

 

   Oh (1992) and Touba (1992, cited in Oxford, 2008) found that adult learners most often use metacognitive strategies for planning, organizing and evaluating their own L2 learning.

 

 

e. Affective strategies and L2 study

Affective strategies deal with how to control fear and anxiety when speaking English via, for example, relaxation, writing down feelings, talking to other people about feelings and so on. Memory and affective studies were found to be the least used strategies by Yang (2007) in his study of Taiwanese students. Likewise, Park (1994, cited in Oxford, 1996) found affective and compensatory strategies to be least used in his study using university students in Korea.

 

 

   Green & Oxford (1995, cited in Oxford, 1996) found affective and memory categories to have no significant bearing upon proficiency level. Lee & Oxford (2008) found affective strategies, followed by social strategies were least used by 1,000 Korean, middle school, high school and university students.

 

 

f. Social strategies and L2 study

Some studies found social strategies to be the least used by Chinese and Japanese students (Noguchi, 1991, cited in Oxford, 1996; Politzer and McGroarty, 1985), however Yang (2007) found social strategies to be the second highest used category amongst junior college students in Taiwan after compensation strategies.

 

 

3. This study’s goals

This paper addresses the following questions: to what extent are university students who study English and Chinese at three Korean university using study strategies as measured by the SILL; what relationship exists between study strategies and institute attendance; and how those variables relate to gender, parents educational level, self-reported self-confidence, and university students’ average foreign language class grades?

 

 

II. METHODS

Korean University students ( N = 276) studying EFL and CFL at Konyang University, Hankook University of Foreign Studies and Sangmyung University were administered two questionnaires, the Student Institute Survey Questionnaire (prepared by authors), and a Korean language translation of the Strategy Inventory for Language Learning (SILL Version 7.0, ESL/EFL, Oxford, 1990). Oxford (1996) reported that reliability of the ESL/EFL SILL goes down when administered in the target language, thus, in this study the SILL was administered in the test groups’ native language, Korean. This Korean language SILL was translated by Professor Kim Jae-Shin (2008, Konyang University).

 

 

   Response sheets from the Student Institute Survey Questionnaire were divided into two groups, those that attended foreign language institutes during their university years and those that did not. Results from the two groups’ SILLS were further divided into EFL and CFL groups.  These results were then analyzed for overall strategy use, and the six strategy categories (memory, cognitive, compensatory…) according to gender, parents’ educational level, self-reported self confidence, and grades in foreign language classes.  Data was analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) to determine significant differences and correlations between the use of language learning strategies by the sample groups.

 

III. RESULTS

First, the SILL and Institute Questionnaire variables were analyzed with EFL and CFL student responses combined as both groups consist of Korean students.  To test for differences between EFL and CFL student strategy use in relationship to all variables, EFL and CFL student SILL and questionnaire responses were then analyzed separately. A table of the overall results summary (not including the six SILL category results) is presented in Appendix 1.

 

 

1. STUDENT GROUPS (EFL & CFL) COMBINED

a. Institute attendance and strategy use

Results indicate a significant difference in overall strategy use between university students that attended institutes at some time in their life and those that did not, t (214) = 3.82, p = .000. Average strategy use by university students that attended institutes (M = 3.09) was significantly greater than those that didn’t attend institutes (M = 2.76). The strongest significant difference in SILL strategy categories between students that attended institutes and those that didn’t was for social strategies (t = 4.33, p = .000), while the lowest difference was for memory strategies (t = 2.07, p = .039). 

 

[Table 1, here]

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Strategy

Category

EFL & CFL

Student groups combined

EFL student results

CFL student results

Overall

means

Went to

institute

Means

No

 Institute

Means

Went to

institute Means

No Institute

Means

Went to

institute Means

No Institute

Means

A. Memory

2.6956

N = 225

2.7835

N = 176

2.6077

N = 49

2.5438

N = 66

2.5018

N = 31

2.9273

N = 110

2.8472

N = 48

B. Cognitive

2.8634

N = 224

2.9975

N = 173

2.7292

N = 48

2.7879

N = 65

2.6024

N = 30

3.1237

N = 108

2.9807

N = 48

C. Compen.

3.35865

N = 225

3.4962

N = 176

3.2211

N = 49

3.4495

N = 66

3.1290

N = 31

3.5242

N = 110

3.3264

N = 48

D. Meta-

Cognitive

3.1001

N = 222

3.3321

N = 174

2.8681

N = 48

3.1726

N = 65

2.8136

N = 31

3.4271

N = 109

3.1868

N = 47

E. Affective

2.6935

N = 225

2.8598

N = 176

2.5272

N = 49

2.7551

N = 66

2.4570

N = 31

2.9227

N = 110

2.8438

N = 48

F.

Social

3.0231

N = 221

3.3081

N = 172

2.7381

N = 49

3.0215

N = 62

2.5108

N = 31

3.4697

N = 110

3.2021

N = 47

Overall

   Means

2.9282

N = 214

3.0934

N = 167

2.7630

N = 47

2.8743

N = 60

2.6567

N = 30

3.2163

N = 107

3.0383

N = 46

Table 1 Institute variable, CFL & EFL groups and SILL category mean results

 

 

b. Gender and strategy use

No significant differences were found between male and female overall strategy use, t (255) = .28, p = .773) or between any of the six strategy categories in the combined EFL/CFL analysis. 

 

 

c. Father’s education and strategy use

Significant correlations were found between father’s educational level and overall mean strategy use (r = .165, significant at 0.01 level, 2 tailed, N = 248).

 

 

   Results also showed significant differences above 95% between two levels of father’s education (graduated from high school and four-year university degree), overall study strategy means, t (208) = 2.31, p = .022, and two categories of study strategy use,  compensatory, t (208) = 3.33, p = .001 and social t (208) = 2.40, p = .017. Use of metacognitive strategies came in third place t (208) = 1.85, p = .065, followed by cognitive, memory and affective strategies respectively.

 

 

d. Mother’s education and strategy use

No significant differences were found between mother’s educational level and strategy use t (216) = 1.56, p = .119, in the combined EFL/CFL group. However the use of compensatory strategies was found to be borderline significantly different according to mother’s educational level, t = 2.56, p = .011.  In addition, a strong correlation was found between mother’s level of education and strategy use on the SILL, r = .168, p< 0.01. 

 

 

e. Students self reported grades and strategy use

Students self-reported grades strongly correlated with overall strategy use means (r = .208, significant at the 0.01 level, two tailed, N= 263), as well as with five of the six categories of strategy use: memory strategy use (.132, .05 significant level, N =  254), cognitive strategy use (r = .203, significant at the 0.01 level, 2 tailed, N = 250), compensatory strategy use (r = .163, significant at 0.01 level, 2 tailed, N =  254), metacognitive strategy use (r = .225, significant at the 0.01 level, N = 251) and social strategy use (r = .165, significant at the 0.01 level, 2 tailed, N=248). Only affective study strategies were not found to be significantly correlated with students self-reported grades in this study (r = .070, p< .268, N = 254).

 

 

   No significant correlation was found between students self-reported grades in foreign language studies and institute attendance. (r = .075, p< .288, N  = 254).

 

f. Students self-reported self-confidence and strategy use

Results indicate there were highly significant differences in intensity of strategy use between higher and lower levels of students self-reported self-confidence in all study strategy categories above the 99% significance level (overall t = 4.01, p = .000), except affective strategies, which yielded a t score of 1.32 (p = .193).

 

 

   A very strong Pearson’s Correlation was found between students’ self-reported confidence and intensity of study strategy use, r = .406, significant above the 0.01 level, (2-tailed, N = 241).

 

 

g. Students’ year in the university

In an examination of students first and second years at their universities, the overall t score was 1.29, barely significant at the .055 level (N = 189). Three categories of the SILL were found to be significantly different between freshman and sophomore years: compensatory, t (189) = 2.85, p = .005, metacognitive, t (189) = 3.22, p = .001 and affective, t (189) = 2.39, p = .018.Analysis of second, third and fourth year strategy use did not reveal significant differences or correlations.

 

 

h. Hours per week studying foreign language outside of university and institutes

A strong correlation was found between hours per week studying and overall mean study strategy intensity, r = .345, significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed, N = 252).

 

 

i.   Most and least commonly used individual strategies

The single most commonly used SILL strategy used in this study by students was strategy 45, “If I do not understand something in English, I ask the other person to slow down or say it again,” a social strategy (M = 3.87). The second most commonly used strategy was strategy 10, “I write or say new English words several times,” a strategy in the cognitive category (M = 3.85). The following are the next most commonly used SILL strategies: “When I can’t think of a word in conversation, I use gestures,” (SILL number 25, compensatory) followed by SILL number 38: “I think about my progress in learning English,” or “…Chinese” for CFL students, a metacognitive strategy).

 

 

   The least frequently used strategies were: SILL number 6, “I use flash cards to remember new English words (M = 1.76), a memory strategy, followed by number 43, “I write down my feelings in a language learning diary,” (M = 2.00) an affective strategy and number 16, “I read for pleasure in English,” (M = 2.11) a cognitive strategy. 

 

 

2. EFL STUDENTS RESULTS

a. Institute attendance and strategy use

The results indicate that there was a significant difference in study strategy use between EFL students that attended institutes and those that didn’t, t (91) = 2.28, p = .025 and a weak correlation of .198, p<0.061.

 

 

b. Gender and strategy use

The results indicate a highly significant difference in overall strategy use between males and females in the EFL group, with female students using strategies with greater intensity, t (109) = 2.69, p = .008. The strongest significant difference between male and female EFL students in the categories of the SILL was in the memory category, t = 2.46, p = .015, followed by cognitive and affective categories, t = 1.91, p = .058, and t = 1.87, p = .064, respectively. The subcategory with the lowest significant differences between EFL males and females’ strategy use was metacognitive, t = 1.65, p = .100.

 

 

   There was also a significant correlation between male and female use of different strategies on the SILL, r = 2.52, significant at the .008 level.

 

 

c. Father’s education and strategy use

A significant correlation was found between father’s level of education and EFL students’ overall strategy use, r = .198, p< 0.05.

 

 

d. Mother’s education and strategy use

No correlation was found in the EFL group between mother’s level of education and EFL students’ overall strategy use.

 

 

e. Students self reported grades and strategy use

No significant correlations were found between EFL students self-reported grades in foreign language classes and overall strategy use or strategy subcategory use.

 

 

f. Students self-reported self confidence and strategy use

The correlation between EFL students self-reported self confidence and overall strategy use was significant at the .01 level, r = .328, p< 0.01.

 

 

g. Students’ year in the university

The only significant differences in strategy use by students’ year in the university in the EFL sample were found between freshman and sophomore year students. The results indicate that there was a highly significant difference in intensity of overall strategy use between freshman and sophomore EFL students, t (92) = 2.45, p = .016, with sophomore students using significantly more strategies in all categories of the SILL. In regards to the categories, the strongest significant differences between freshmen and sophomores were found in meta-cognitive and compensatory strategies, t = 3.09, p = .003 and t = 2.95, p = .004, respectively. The smallest difference between use of strategy categories between freshman and sophomores was in the use of memory strategies t = .250, p = .803. The least used strategy category was freshman use of affective strategies (M = 2.55), while the highest mean found in a strategy category was sophomore use of compensatory strategies (M = 3.51).

 

 

   No significant correlations were found between students’ year in the university and overall strategy use in the EFL group.

 

 

h. Hours per week studying foreign language outside of university and institutes

The results indicate that there was a highly significant difference in study strategy intensity between students that spent more time studying (13 hours per week) outside the classroom and those that spent less time studying outside the classroom (two hours per week), t (54) = 4.10, p = .000. There was a very strong correlation between time spent studying and study strategy intensity means, r = .321, significant at the .01 level.

 

 

   A correlational analysis between institute attendance and time spent studying revealed there was no correlation between these two variables (r = .062, p<.546). Though foreign language study outside the classroom appears to relate strongly with study strategy intensity, and institute attendance relates strongly with study strategy intensity, these factors appear to be independent.

 

 

3. CFL STUDENT RESULTS

a. Institute attendance and strategy use

Results indicate there was a significant difference in strategy use between students that attended an institute and those that didn’t, t (153) = 1.99, p = .049. There was also a significant correlation between CFL students’ attendance at a foreign language institute, and study strategy use, r = .211, significant at the .05 level.

 

 

b. Gender and strategy use

Unlike the EFL group, there was no significant correlation between gender and study strategy use in the CFL group, r = .081, p< .323, N = 153.

 

 

c. Father’s education and strategy use

Unlike the EFL group, there was no significant correlation between father’s level of education and strategy use, r = .120, p< 0.158, N = 153.

 

 

d. Mother’s education and strategy use

Unlike the EFL group, there was a significant correlation between mother’s level of education and strategy use, r = .187, significant at the .05 level, N = 153. Though there wasn’t a significant difference between CFL students overall strategy use based on level of mother’s education, t (120) = 1.09, p = .275, a significant difference was found in the compensatory category (t = 2.52, p = .013) and a borderline significant difference was also found in cognitive (t = 1.92, p = .057) and affective (t = 1.73, p = .085) strategy categories. Significant correlations were found between mother’s education and cognitive strategies (.05 level), compensation (.01 level), and metacognitive (.05 level) strategies.

 

 

e. Students self reported grades and strategy use

There was a highly significant correlation between CFL students’ self-reported grades and mean strategy use, r = .301, significant at the .01 level.

 

 

f. Students self-reported self confidence and strategy use

Like the EFL group, there was a strong positive correlation between self-reported self-confidence and study strategy use, r = .433, significant at the .01 level, N = 153. There was also a very strong correlation in the CFL group between confidence and hours of target language study outside the classroom, r = .374, p< .000, N = 152.

 

 

g. Students’ year in the university

Unlike the EFL group, a significant correlation was found between year of university study and study strategy use, r = .202, significant at the .05 level, N = 153.

 

 

h. Hours per week studying foreign language outside of university and institutes

There was a borderline significant difference t (86) = 1.82, p = .072, in student SILL means between students who reported studying an average of two hours per week and students who reported studying 8.5 hours per week. There was a significant difference, t (73) = 4.38, p. = .000, in SILL means between students who reported studying two hours per week and students who reported studying 13 hours per week.  There was a very strong correlation (r = .337,  p<.000, N = 152) between hours per week studying a foreign language and study strategy mean use although there was a rather radical dip in study strategy usage at the 18.5 hours per week level in the CFL group.

 

 

IV. DISCUSSION

1. Overview of strategy use

As Table 1 illustrates, the overall Grand Mean for all students in this study was 2.9 (on a scale of 1 – 5). Compensatory strategies were most used by this total sample of Korean students (M = 3.36), consistent with results by Lee (2003) and Oxford (2008) in their studies on Korean students’ study strategy use. Oxford (2008) wrote that: “Since 1969, Korean students have taken multiple-choice entrance examinations, equivalent to the Scholastic Aptitude Test in the U.S. and we speculate that such examinations might promote compensatory strategies for guessing the right choice from the context, even if the details are not fully understood.” Yang (2007) and Hsu (2003) also found that compensatory strategies were most frequently used in their studies.  Regarding the primary use of compensatory strategies, Yang wrote: “It is only natural for students to make greater use of compensation strategies as these can allow them to guess the meaning of what they have heard or read or to remain in the conversation despite their limited grammatical and vocabulary knowledge.” This study found metacognitive to be the second most used strategy (M = 3.10), also consistent with Oxford (2008). 

 

 

   Following that similarity however, the results from this study diverged from Oxfords, but were consistent with most contemporary research on study strategy use in Asia (see Table 2).

 

This study (South Korea)

Oxford, 2008

(South Korea)

Yang, 2007

China

Hsu, 2003

Taiwan

Lengkanawati, 2004

Indonesia

Compensation

Compensation

Compensation

Compensation

Memory

Metacognitive

Metacognitive

Social

Affective

Cognitive

Social

Cognitive

Cognitive

Social

Compensation

Cognitive

Memory

Metacognitive

Metacognitive

Affective

Affective

Affective

Affective

Cognitive

Social

Memory

Social

Memory

Memory

Metacognitive

Table 2. Most Commonly used strategy categories in five studies in Asia

 

 

   Memory was found to be the least used strategy in this and other recent studies, compared to older research by Politzer and McGroarty (1985); Tyacke & Mendelsohn (1986); Huang & Van Naerrsen (1987); O’Mally and Chamot (1990) and Lengkanawati (2004), which suggested that Asians tend to prefer memorization and other rule-bound strategies.

 

 

   One possible explanation for these differences could be that most of the research that found Asians prefer memory based strategies was done back in the 1980s, when older systems of language teaching were used. In regards to Lengkanawati’s (2004) results, he wrote: “Indonesian students have the habit of rote learning behavior. This behavior has become the cultural habit in studying.”

 

 

   Studies by Chang (1990); Bedell (1993, cited in Oxford, 1996), Yang (1993a, 1993b, cited in Yang, 2007); Biggs, 1994; Cortazi and Jin, 1996; Littlewood (2000) and Lee and Oxford (2008) raise questions about preconceptions regarding cultural influences on learning strategies. Lee and Oxford for example, found that memory strategies were not more frequently used by Korean students.

 

 

2. Variables and strategy use

a. Institute attendance and strategy use

Students that attended private language institutes at sometime in their life demonstrated significantly greater study strategy intensity use in this study, in combined and separate EFL and CFL groups. However, correlations and significant differences do not necessarily imply causation, as some intervening variable such as motivation may explain both increased probability to attend a language institute and greater intensity of study strategy use.

 

 

b. Gender and strategy use

That highly significant differences and a significant correlation in strategy use by males and females should be found in the EFL group but not the CFL group is consistent with contradictory results of other studies. Clearly other variables are at work here.  Numerous investigators have found results similar to the EFL group in this study, i.e. females make more frequent use of strategies than males, i.e. Lee (2003) and Hsu (2008).  In her 1996 study, Oxford cites 14 studies that examined study strategy use and gender, concluding that “results usually favored females as more frequent users of strategies.” However, in her 2008 study conducted with Korean students, she found gender by itself did not affect strategy use significantly. Bedell (cited in Yang, 2007) and Watanabe (cited in Yang, 2007) found that females utilize different patterns of strategy use than males. Bedell (1993) and Green and Oxford (1993, cited in Oxford, 1996) found that males surpass females in some specific strategy items, but not whole categories. 

 

 

c. Father’s education and strategy use

Whereas this study found significant differences between student use of compensatory and social strategies based on their fathers’ education, Hsu (2008) found parents’ education most influenced cognitive and social strategy use. That the EFL group should have a significant correlation between fathers’ level of education and strategy use whereas the CFL group did not, is curious. Hsu (2008) looked at “parents” education as a unity and didn’t differentiate between fathers’ and mothers’ educational levels. He defined educational level as “low” when parents’ education was equal or below senior high school (the same as this study) and “high” when the parents’ education level was Junior college or better, whereas in this study we considered “high” to be a four-year university bachelors degree. In attempting to explain the differences in study strategies resulting from having parents with different educational backgrounds, Hsu wrote: “It is because parents with higher education level respect children’s education more.”

 

 

d. Mother’s education and strategy use

In the combined EFL/CFL analysis, mother’s education had a strong correlation with student strategy use. That the CFL group produced a significant correlation between mother’s level of education and strategy use, whereas the EFL group did not, while the EFL group showed a correlation between father’s education and strategy use, but the CFL group did not, warrants further research. 

 

 

   The answer may lie in Korea’s traditional culture. For example, older generation people in Korea tend to adhere much more closely to traditional values associated with sex-role socialization than younger people. Also, English language is considered the modern way of the future, whereas Chinese is associated with traditional values. Consequently it’s possible that highly educated mothers may consciously or unconsciously encourage their daughters towards learning Chinese language studies and educated fathers may consciously or unconsciously steer their sons towards learning English language.

 

 

e. Students self reported grades and strategy use

This study found significant positive correlations between foreign language class grades at the university and SILL scores, consistent with Park (1994) who used the SILL and TOEFL with 332 Korean university students.  His results indicated that the high strategy use group had TOEFL scores significantly higher than that of the medium strategy use group, though the medium and low strategy use groups only had slight language proficiency differences as measured by the TOEFL. Mullins study (as cited in Oxford, 2008) of language strategy use and English proficiency with 110 Thai students, found significant correlations between compensation and metacognitive strategy use and English ability using course grades and language placement scores. A study by Green (cited in Oxford, 1996) using ESLAT proficiency scores with Spanish speaking EFL students in Puerto Rico found moderate and significant differences in strategy use between higher and lower English proficiency students. Green and Oxford (1995, cited in Oxford, 1996) found that language proficiency level had significant effects on compensatory, cognitive, metacognitive and social strategies.

 

 

f. Students self-reported self confidence and strategy use

Self confidence was the second most highly correlated factor with study strategies obtained in this study. Oxfords 2008 study conducted in Korea included a measure she called “English learning self-image.” This quality was measured by response to the question: “How do you rate your overall English proficiency as compared with the proficiency of other student in your class.” (Italics were used in the questionnaire.) Oxford’s “English learning self-image” question sounds similar to self-confidence in the context of an EFL or CFL class. Oxfords 2008 study found a strong relationship between “English learning self-image” and study strategy use, consistent with findings in this study.

 

 

g. Students’ year in the university

It is curious that the EFL students should show a significant increase between freshman and sophomore years, and then level off for the next three years compared to the CFL students that showed a slow but steady increase throughout their university career. This result may be an anomaly of different admissions standards at the university, or reflection of different cultures of learning (i.e. Cortazzi and Jin, 1996) within the respective departments. Numerous studies have shown correlations between age and study strategy use. For example, Oxford 2008 found significantly higher strategy use in Korean university students compared to high school and middle school students.

 

 

   Conversely, Hsu (2008) found that Taiwanese elementary school children used study strategies with greater intensity in all strategy categories than did two-year junior college students. A number of intervening variables could account for this discrepancy, for example, the age at which the college students started learning English could influence their strategy usage.  Some influence on foreign language study strategy use by the males in this study after the freshman year (irrespective of language studied) could be the result of the mandatory two year military service all young men must undergo at the end of their freshman year (except those who enter an ROTC program). Students with a more traditional philosophical attitude, inculcated in Chinese philosophy (and to some extent language) might recuperate more quickly from the potential trauma of forced military service than someone with expectations of the freedoms that are woven through most modern societies and the international language of English.  Such reasoning is highly speculative and further research is required to see if these results are consistent across South Korea.

 

 

h. Hours per week studying foreign language outside of university and institutes

EFL and CFL groups both produced high correlations between strategy use and study outside the classroom, although the radical dip in strategy usage in the CFL group at the 18.5 hour level was unexpected. It could be that students who felt above average need for study did so because their proficiency level was low, possibly due to limited use of study strategies. EFL strategy use had a minor dip at 5 hours per week of outside the classroom study and a major dip at 9 hours a week of outside the classroom study. There was a peak in strategy use in the 18 hours a week of outside class study in the EFL group.

 

 

   The mean study time of students who attended institutes at some time in life was higher than that of students that never attended an institute (M =  4.0 hours per week vs. 3.4 hours per week), though the differences were not significant. An intervening variable such as motivation could explain the difference. Attending an institute at some time in life probably does not cause greater time in study. However, the motivation to spend more time studying could help “cause” attendance at an institute.

 

 

   In Hsu’s 2008 study he included a factor he referred to as: “Learning experiences outside of school.” Though he didn’t elaborate on the meaning of this phrase, his groups on this factor were divided into “less than one year” and “more than one year.” He may have been referring to institutes, tutoring, independent study or some combination of the three.  Whatever the case, he found significant differences in study strategy use between students with more and less than one year of “learning experience outside of school.”

 

 

3. Suggestions for further research

The results of this study raised several questions. For example, why should mother’s level of education correlate highly with CFL students mean strategy intensity use, but not with EFL students strategy use, and why did fathers educational level correlate highly with EFL students study strategy use, and not with CFL students study strategy use? A replication study needs to be done to ascertain if this pattern is consistent in South Korea, or if it is an anomaly of these students at these three universities. Should these results be consistent across South Korea, answers may be found in cultural attitudes towards Chinese and/or English speaking people.

 

 

   Another question which remains unanswered is: Why was there a correlation between gender and study strategy use in the EFL group, but not the CFL group? Are different personality types attracted to studying different languages?

 

   Likewise, that the EFL students’ mean strategy use leveled off after the freshman year whereas the CFL students showed a slow steady increase.  Again, further research is required to understand the mechanisms for this result.

 

 

   Not much research has been done on motivation as a factor in study strategy use, even though Oxford’s 1990 SILL came complete with a questionnaire that included a motivation measure. Oxford and Nyikos (1989) found motivation and foreign language study strategy use to be related. Oxford, Talbott and Halleck (1990, cited in Oxford, 1996) found a significant relationship between strategy use and motivation. Oxford, Park-Oh, Ito, and Sumrall (1993a, 1993b) found motivation to be a significant factor along with study strategy use in Japanese language learning.  Hsu (2003) found motivation to be the greatest predictor of variance of EFL proficiency in his study.

 

 

   Culhane (2004) cites research by Gardner and Lambert (1959) which identified five motivational attributes affecting L2 acquisition: the learner’s reasons for learning the L2; degree of anomie, or dissatisfaction with one’s place and role in society; level of ethnocentrism; the degree to which the first culture is preferred over the second and attitudes held toward the target language and culture. Research on these factors and their interaction with study strategies might help teachers provide curricula and assignments tailored to lift up students’ weak motivational factors and contribute to second language learning. Cross factor analysis between Gardner and Lamberts five motivational attributes and the independent variables in this study could produce valuable insights and answers to bigger questions regarding foreign language learning in Asia.

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

REFERENCES

Altan, M.Z. (2006). Nationality and language learning strategies of EFL-major university students. In The Study of Second Language Acquisition in the Asian Context. Paul Robertson and Roger Nunn (eds.), 250-264. Seoul: Asian EFL Journal Press.

 

Biggs, J. (1994). What are effective schools? Lessons from East and West.  Australian Educational Researcher 21(1): 19-40.

 

Chang, S.J. (1990). A study of the language learning behaviors of Chinese students at the University of Georgia and the relation of those behaviors to oral proficiency and other factors.  Dissertation Abstracts International 52: 450A.

 

Cortazzi, M. and Jin, L. (1996). Cultures of learning: Language classroom in China.  In Society and the language classroom, H. Coleman (ed.), 169-206. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

 

Culhane, S. F. (2004). An intercultural interaction model: Acculturation attitudes in second language acquisition. Electronic Journal of Foreign Language Teaching 1 (1): 50-61. Retrieved August 28, 2008, from: http://e-flt.nus.edu.sg/v1n12004/culhane.htm

 

Does Korea gain from being a 'Republic of English?’ (2007, May 2) Choson Ilbo, Retrieved August 25, 2008, from http://english.chosun.com/w21data/html/news/200705/200705020009.html

 

Hsu, S.C. (2003). A study of business English learning strategies.  Nanya Journal 23: 83-94.

 

Hsu, S.C. (2008). English learning strategy use by elementary school students. Nanya Journal 27: 129-142.  Retrieved August 17, 2008, from: http://web.nanya.edu.tw/acof/acpu/word/vol27/960702.pdf 

 

Huang, X.-H., and Van Naerrsen, M. (1987). Learning strategies for oral communication.  Applied Linguistics 8: 287-307.

 

Lee K.O. (2003). The relationship of school year, sex and proficiency on the use of learning strategies in learning English of Korean junior high school students. Asian EFL Journal 5 (3): Article 4.

 

Lee, K. R., and Oxford, R. (2008). Understanding EFL learners’ strategy use and strategy awareness, Asian EFL Journal 10 (1): Article 1.

 

Lengkanawati, N.S. (2004). How learners from different cultural backgrounds learn a foreign language.  Asian EFL Journal 6 (1): Article 8. 

 

Littlewood, W. (2000). Do Asian students really want to listen and obey? ELT Journal 54 (1): 31-36.

 

OECD. (2007). Executive Summary; The Program for International Student Assessment. Retrieved August 22, 2008, from http://www.pisa.oecd.org/dataoecd/15/13/39725224.pdf

 

Oh, J. (1992). Learning strategies used by university EFL students in Korea.  Language Teaching 1: 3-53.

 

O’Malley, J.M., and Chamot, A.O. (1990).  Learning strategies in second language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

 

Oxford, R.L. (1985). A new taxonomy of second language learning strategies. Washington, D.C: Center for Applied Linguistics.

 

Oxford, R.L. (1989). Use of language learning strategies: A synthesis of studies with implications for strategy training.  System 17: 235-247.

 

Oxford, R.L. (1990). Language learning strategies: What every teacher should know. 293-300. New York: Newbury House.

 

Oxford, R.L. (1996). Employing a questionnaire to assess the use of language learning strategies, Applied Language Learning 7: 25 – 45.  Retrieved August 14, 2008, from http://www.dliflc.edu/academics/academic_materials/all/ALLissues/all7.pdf

 

Oxford, R.L., and Nyikos, M. (1989). Variables affecting choice of language learning strategies by university students. Modern Language Journal 73 (2): 291-300.

 

Oxford, R.L., Park-Oh, Y., Ito, S., and Sumrall, M. (1993a). Factors affecting achievement in a satellite-delivered Japanese language program. American Journal of Distance Education 7: 10-25.

 

Oxford, R.L., Park-Oh, Y., Ito, S., and Sumrall, M. (1993b). Learning a language by satellite: What influences achievement? System 21 (1): 31-48.

 

Politzer, R., and McGroarty, M. (1985). An exploratory study of learning behaviors and their relationship to gains in linguistic and communicative competence. TESOL Quarterly 19: 103-124.

 

Ruben, J. (1975). What the good language learner can teach us. TESOL Quarterly 91: 45-51.

 

Song, S.-H. (2008, August 19).  English service leaves room for improvement, Korea Herald, Analysis and Feature, p. 4.

 

Tyacke, M. and Mendelsohn, D. (1986). Student needs: Cognitive as well as communicative.  TESL Canada Journal 1: 177-198.

 

U.S. Department of Education 2007: National Center for Educational Statistics, NCES 2008–016. Retrieved September 1, 2008, from http://nces.ed.gov//pubs2008/2008016.pdf

 

Wharton, G. (2000). Language learning strategy use of bilingual foreign language learners in Singapore.  Language Learning 50 (2): 203-243.

 

World Bank (2008). World Development Indicators Database, World Bank, July 1, 2008.

Retrieved September 1, 2008, from

http://siteresources.worldbank.org/DATASTATISTICS/Resources/GDP.pdf.

 

Yang, M.N. (2007). Language learning strategies for junior college students in Taiwan: Investigating ethnicity and proficiency, Asian EFL Journal 9 (2): Article 3.

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Appendix 1 Results summary by sample group, variable, overall significance and statistic*

 

EFL & CFL COMBINED

EFL STUDENT RESULTS

CFL STUDENT RESULTS

a. Institute attendance and strategy use

Sig. diff.

 

Sig.

corre-

lation

t = 3.82

p = .000

r = .200

p< .01

Sig. diff.

 

Weak

corre-

lation

t = 2.28

p = .025

r = .198

p< 0.061

Sig. diff.

 

Sig. corre-

lation

t = 1.99

p = .049

r = .211

p< 0.05

b. Gender and strategy use

No

sig. diff.

No corre-

lation

t = .288

p = .773

r = .020

p< .752

Sig. diff.

 

Strong

corre-

lation

t = 2.69

p = .008

r = .252 p< 0.008

No sig.

diff.

No corre-lation

t = 1.10

p = .271

r = .081

p< 0.323

c. Father’s education and strategy use

Sig. diff.

 

Strong corre-lation

t = 2.31

p = .022

r = .165

p< 0.01

Sig. diff.

 

Sig. corre-lation

t = 2.10

p = .038

r = .198 p< 0.05

No sig.

diff.

No corre-lation

t = 1.26

p = .210

r = .120

p< 0.158

d. Mother’s education and strategy use

No

sig. diff.

Strong

corre-

lation

t = 1.56

p = .119

r = .168

p< 0.01

No sig.

diff.

No corre-lation

t = 1.34

p = .184

r = .127 p<  0.192

No sig. diff.

Sig. corre-lation

t = 1.09

p = .275

r = .187

p< 0.05

e. Students’ self-reported grades and strategy use

Sig. diff.

 

Strong

corre-lation

t = 3.27

p = .002

r = .208

p< 0.01

No corre-lation

r = .087

p< 0.405

Strong corre-lation

r = .301

p< 0.01

f. Students’ self-reported self-confidence and strategy use

Sig. diff.

 

Strong

corr.

t = 4.01

p = .000

r = .406

p< 0.00

Strong corre-lation

r = .328

p< 0.01

Strong corre-lation

r = .433

p< 0.00

g. Students year in the university(1-2)

Sig. diff.

t = 1.29

p = .055

Sig. diff.

 

t = 2.45

p = .016

Strong corre-lation

r = .202

p< 0.05

h. Hours per week studying foreign language outside of classroom

Sig. diff.

 

Strong corre-lation

t = 4.05

p = .000

r = .345

p< 0.00

Sig. diff.

 

Strong

corre-

lation

t = 4.10

p = .000

r = .321

p< .01

Sig diff.

 

Strong

corre-

lation

t = 4.38

p = .000

r = .337

p< .0.00

* For SILL category results, see text.